MIT: Some decisions arouse far more anxiety than others. Among the most
anxiety-provoking are those that involve options with both positive and
negative elements, such choosing to take a higher-paying job in a city
far from family and friends, versus choosing to stay put with less pay. MIT researchers have now identified a neural circuit that appears to
underlie decision-making in this type of situation, which is known as
approach-avoidance conflict. The findings could help researchers to
discover new ways to treat psychiatric disorders that feature impaired
decision-making, such as depression, schizophrenia, and borderline
personality disorder.
“In order to create a treatment for these types of disorders, we need
to understand how the decision-making process is working,” says
Alexander Friedman, a research scientist at MIT’s McGovern Institute for
Brain Research and the lead author of a paper describing the findings
in the May 28 issue of Cell.
Friedman and colleagues also demonstrated the first step toward
developing possible therapies for these disorders: By manipulating this
circuit in rodents, they were able to transform a preference for
lower-risk, lower-payoff choices to a preference for bigger payoffs
despite their bigger costs.
The paper’s senior author is Ann Graybiel, an MIT Institute Professor
and member of the McGovern Institute. Other authors are postdoc Daigo
Homma, research scientists Leif Gibb and Ken-ichi Amemori,
undergraduates Samuel Rubin and Adam Hood, and technical assistant
Michael Riad.
Making hard choices
The new study grew out of an effort to figure out the role of
striosomes — clusters of cells distributed through the the striatum, a
large brain region involved in coordinating movement and emotion and
implicated in some human disorders. Graybiel discovered striosomes many
years ago, but their function had remained mysterious, in part because
they are so small and deep within the brain that it is difficult to
image them with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).
Previous studies from Graybiel’s lab identified regions of the
brain’s prefrontal cortex that project to striosomes. These regions have
been implicated in processing emotions, so the researchers suspected
that this circuit might also be related to emotion.
To test this idea, the researchers studied mice as they performed
five different types of behavioral tasks, including an
approach-avoidance scenario. In that situation, rats running a maze had
to choose between one option that included strong chocolate, which they
like, and bright light, which they don’t, and an option with dimmer
light but weaker chocolate.
When humans are forced to make these kinds of cost-benefit decisions,
they usually experience anxiety, which influences the choices they
make. “This type of task is potentially very relevant to anxiety
disorders,” Gibb says. “If we could learn more about this circuitry,
maybe we could help people with those disorders.”
The researchers also tested rats in four other scenarios in which the choices were easier and less fraught with anxiety.
“By comparing performance in these five tasks, we could look at
cost-benefit decision-making versus other types of decision-making,
allowing us to reach the conclusion that cost-benefit decision-making is
unique,” Friedman says.
Using optogenetics, which allowed them to turn cortical input to the
striosomes on or off by shining light on the cortical cells, the
researchers found that the circuit connecting the cortex to the
striosomes plays a causal role in influencing decisions in the
approach-avoidance task, but none at all in other types of
decision-making.
When the researchers shut off input to the striosomes from the
cortex, they found that the rats began choosing the high-risk,
high-reward option as much as 20 percent more often than they had
previously chosen it. If the researchers stimulated input to the
striosomes, the rats began choosing the high-cost, high-reward option
less often.
Paul Glimcher, a professor of physiology and neuroscience at New York
University, describes the study as a “masterpiece” and says he is
particularly impressed by the use of a new technology, optogenetics, to
solve a longstanding mystery. The study also opens up the possibility of
studying striosome function in other types of decision-making, he adds.
“This cracks the 20-year puzzle that [Graybiel] wrote — what do the
striosomes do?” says Glimcher, who was not part of the research team.
“In 10 years we will have a much more complete picture, of which this
paper is the foundational stone. She has demonstrated that we can answer
this question, and answered it in one area. A lot of labs will now take
this up and resolve it in other areas.”
Emotional gatekeeper
The findings suggest that the striatum, and the striosomes in
particular, may act as a gatekeeper that absorbs sensory and emotional
information coming from the cortex and integrates it to produce a
decision on how to react, the researchers say.
That gatekeeper circuit also appears to include a part of the
midbrain called the substantia nigra, which has dopamine-containing
cells that play an important role in motivation and movement. The
researchers believe that when activated by input from the striosomes,
these substantia nigra cells produce a long-term effect on an animal or
human patient’s decision-making attitudes.
“We would so like to find a way to use these findings to relieve
anxiety disorder, and other disorders in which mood and emotion are
affected,” Graybiel says. “That kind of work has a real priority to it.”
In addition to pursuing possible treatments for anxiety disorders,
the researchers are now trying to better understand the role of the
dopamine-containing substantia nigra cells in this circuit, which plays a
critical role in Parkinson’s disease and may also be involved in
related disorders.