UNC: Using a weak electric current in an attempt to boost brainpower or
treat conditions has become popular among scientists and
do-it-yourselfers, but a new UNC School of Medicine study shows that
using the most common form of electric brain stimulation had a
statistically significant detrimental effect on IQ scores. Published in the journal Behavioural Brain Research, the study
adds to the increasing amount of literature showing that transcranial
direct current stimulation – tDCS – has mixed results when it comes to
cognitive enhancement.
“It would be wonderful if we could use tDCS to enhance cognition
because then we could potentially use it to treat cognitive impairment
in psychiatric illnesses,” said Flavio Frohlich, PhD, study senior
author and assistant professor of psychiatry, cell biology and
physiology, biomedical engineering, and neurology. “So, this study is
bad news. Yet, the finding makes sense. It means that some of the most
sophisticated things the brain can do, in terms of cognition, can’t
necessarily be altered with just a constant electric current.”
Frohlich, though, said that using less common alternating current
stimulation – so-called tACS – could be a better approach, one that he
has been investigating. Earlier this year, Frohlich’s lab found that tACS significantly boosted creativity,
likely because he used it to target the brain’s natural electrical
alpha oscillations, which have been implicated in creative thought.
With tDCS, scientists don’t target these brain waves, which represent
neuronal patterns of communication throughout regions of the brain.
Instead, they use tDCS to target brain structures, such particular
regions of the cortex.
The tDCS boom started in 2000, when German scientists published a
paper showing that tDCS could change the excitability of neurons in the
motor cortex – the brain region that controls voluntary body movement.
Since then, there’s been an explosion of tDCS studies to try to make
neurons more active or less active and therefore change outcomes for a
variety of brain functions, such as working memory and cognitive acuity,
and for illnesses, such as depression and schizophrenia.
But Frohlich said that some of the studies that have made waves were
poorly designed. Some studies were not properly double-blinded or
properly placebo controlled. Other studies were very small – less than
10 people.
A recent meta-analysis
of a large number of tDCS papers showed that tDCS is far from a magic
pill for cognitive enhancement or brain-related health conditions.
“Aside from stimulating the motor cortex, which has very exciting
implications for stroke rehabilitation, I think the jury is still out on
tDCS,” said Frohlich, who is a member of the UNC Neuroscience Center.
In the Behavioural Brain Research study, Frohlich’s team –
including graduate student Kristin Sellers, the paper’s first author –
recruited 40 healthy adults, each of whom took the standard WAIS-IV
intelligence test, which is the most common and well-validated test of
IQ. It includes tests for verbal comprehension, perceptional reasoning,
working memory, and processing speed.
A week later, Frohlich’s team divided the participants into two
groups. Electrodes were placed on each side of each participant’s scalp,
under which sat the frontal cortex. Duke University collaborator and
co-author Angel Peterchev, PhD, created imaging simulations to ensure
Frohlich’s team targeted the same parts of the cortex that previous tDCS
studies had targeted.
Then the placebo group received sham stimulation – a brief electrical
current, which led participants to think they had been receiving the
full tDCS. The other participants received the standard tDCS for twenty
minutes – a weak electrical current of 2 millioamperes.
All participants then retook the IQ tests. Frohlich expected that
most, if not all, IQ scores would improve because of the practice
effect, but that tDCS would not markedly improve scores.
Frohlich’s team did find that all scores improved. Surprisingly, though, the participants who did not
receive tDCS saw their IQ scores increase by ten points, whereas
participants who received tDCS saw their IQ scores increase by just shy
of six points, on average.
When Frohlich and colleagues analyzed the test scores, they saw that
the scores for three of the four main kinds of cognitive tests were very
similar between the two groups of participants. But the scores for
perceptual reasoning were much lower among people who underwent tDCS.
Perceptual reasoning tests fluid intelligence, which is defined as
the ability to think logically and apply innovative problem solving to
new problems.
Within the category of perceptual reasoning, the researchers saw the
biggest differences in the subcategory of matrix reasoning – when
participants viewed two groups of symbols and had to find the one symbol
missing from the other group.
Frohlich emphasized, “Our findings do not preclude the possibility
that other tDCS paradigms may be less harmful or even beneficial.
However, it is time to make sure that everybody uses gold standard,
placebo-controlled, double-blind study designs. Also, our study
demonstrates the importance of more research on how stimulation
interacts with brain activity.”
Frohlich stressed that the scientific community should be careful not
to create simplistic storylines about tDCS being a ‘magic pill’ for
many brain-related conditions. “There could be dangerous consequences,
especially if tDCS is used daily,” he said. “Ours was an acute study. We
don’t know what the long-term effects are. There is so much more we
need to understand before tDCS is ready for home use without medical
supervision.”
Frohlich added, “I think our study demonstrates that we need to think
of smarter ways to engage the brain to really target the specific brain
dynamics involved in what we want to improve, such as cognition for
people with depression or schizophrenia. I think tACS is an option, as
well as more sophisticated modalities we’ve yet to develop.”